Self Studies

Writing Test 20

Result Self Studies

Writing Test 20
  • Score

    -

    out of -
  • Rank

    -

    out of -
TIME Taken - -
Self Studies

SHARING IS CARING

If our Website helped you a little, then kindly spread our voice using Social Networks. Spread our word to your readers, friends, teachers, students & all those close ones who deserve to know what you know now.

Self Studies Self Studies
Weekly Quiz Competition
  • Question 1
    1 / -0

    Directions For Questions

    Both passages discuss the issue of the intelligence of dogs.
    Passage 1 was adapted from a 2001 book on animal
    intelligence. Passage 2 was written in 2001 by a dog
    trainer and writer.

    Passage 1
    It was no accident that nineteenth-century naturalist
    Charles Darwin strove to connect the mentality and
    emotionality of people with that of dogs, rather than, say,
    doves or horses. Neither his theory of evolution nor any
    general understanding of biology demanded that he pref-
    erentially underline our similarity to dogs over other
    species. But politically and emotionally, the choice was
    inevitable for an English gentleman who had set himself
    the task of making the idea of evolutionary continuity
    palatable. Darwin wrote that "dogs possess something
    very similar to a conscience. They certainly possess
    some power of self-command. . . . Dogs have long been
    accepted as the very type of fidelity and obedience."
    Darwin was not alone in his beliefs that dogs possess
    human virtues. The characteristics of loyalty and obedience,
    coupled with an expressive face and body, can account
    for why dogs are such popular and valued pets in many
    cultures. Depending on the breed and the individual, dogs
    can be noble, charming, affectionate, and reliable. But
    while all dog owners should rightly appreciate these and
    other endearing traits in their pets, nothing says that the
    cleverness of a highly intelligent primate such as a chim-
    panzee is part of the package. Scientists generally believe
    the reasoning abilities of chimps to be considerably greater
    than that of dogs. But many people nonetheless believe that
    dogs are smarter than chimps precisely because of our
    familiarity and emotional ties with the dogs that we love.
    We apply the same secret rules to our fellow humans: the
    old in-group, out-group story. People in your in-group are
    those who are similar to you, either because they belong
    to the same organizations as you, or enjoy the same
    activities, or, and this is the kicker, because they are simply
    around more often. Dogs, because of their proximity to
    their owners, are definitely in. The intensity of our
    relationship with dogs causes us, quite naturally, to imbue
    them with high-level mental abilities, whether they have
    earned those extra intelligence points or not. We like them,
    so we think well of them.

    Passage 2
    Every dog trainer that I know had the same childhood, a
    childhood filled with the brilliant, heroic dogs of literature.
    We read about dogs who regularly traveled thousands of
    miles to be reunited with owners who somehow misplaced
    them, repeatedly saved people from certain death, and
    continually exhibited a better grasp of strategic problem-
    solving than the average Ph.D. In the preface to one of his
    many dog stories, S. P. Meek a bit shamefacedly remarked
    that in writing of dogs "I endeavored to hold these heroes
    down to the level of canine intelligence, and to make them,
    above all, believable. If at times I seem to have made them
    show supercanine intelligence, it is because my enthusiasm
    has run away with me." We forgave him, of course.
    It was something of a shock, therefore, to discover
    how the learning theory "experts" believed dogs think
    and learn. I was told that dogs, unlike chimpanzees, have
    no real reasoning ability. Dogs don't think: rather, they
    learn to avoid the unpleasant (negative reinforcement), seek
    the pleasant (positive reinforcement), or some combination
    of the two. To contend otherwise was to be guilty of the sin
    of anthropomorphizing, the attribution to an animal of
    motivations and consciousness that only a human being
    could possess.
    Yet as a dog trainer, I find myself siding more with the
    Meeks than I do with the learning theorists: nobody could
    believe dispassionately in the totality of positive and nega-
    tive reinforcement after seeing the pure intelligence shining
    in the face of a border collie intent upon helping a shepherd
    herd sheep. Dogs do think and reason. Granted, a dog might
    not be able to run a maze as quickly as a chimp. But a dog
    outshines any other animal that I know in the ability to
    work willingly with a human being, to communicate with
    a puzzling creature who often makes incomprehensible
    demands. Researchers have increasingly come to view
    intelligence as a complex collection of mental abilities
    that cannot be fully captured in any simple way. Dogs
    are geniuses at being useful, and it is this usefulness
    that we admire when we praise their intelligence. As
    Jonica Newby, a specialist in animal-human interaction,
    writes, "In some ways intelligence is a matter of match-
    ing behavior to environment. To compare intelligence
    in creatures that have evolved differently is a bit like
    deciding which has hit upon the best mode of travel: the
    dolphin or the horse." And it is dogs, not chimps, who
    possess the most helpful mode of travel for human beings.

    ...view full instructions

    In Passage 2, lines 67-68 ("Granted . . . chimp") principally serve to
  • Question 2
    1 / -0
    The sales assistant arranged the gems on the counter, he proceeded to tell us about the origins of each stone.
  • Question 3
    1 / -0

    Directions For Questions

    Both passages discuss the issue of the intelligence of dogs.
    Passage 1 was adapted from a 2001 book on animal
    intelligence. Passage 2 was written in 2001 by a dog
    trainer and writer.

    Passage 1
    It was no accident that nineteenth-century naturalist
    Charles Darwin strove to connect the mentality and
    emotionality of people with that of dogs, rather than, say,
    doves or horses. Neither his theory of evolution nor any
    general understanding of biology demanded that he pref-
    erentially underline our similarity to dogs over other
    species. But politically and emotionally, the choice was
    inevitable for an English gentleman who had set himself
    the task of making the idea of evolutionary continuity
    palatable. Darwin wrote that "dogs possess something
    very similar to a conscience. They certainly possess
    some power of self-command. . . . Dogs have long been
    accepted as the very type of fidelity and obedience."
    Darwin was not alone in his beliefs that dogs possess
    human virtues. The characteristics of loyalty and obedience,
    coupled with an expressive face and body, can account
    for why dogs are such popular and valued pets in many
    cultures. Depending on the breed and the individual, dogs
    can be noble, charming, affectionate, and reliable. But
    while all dog owners should rightly appreciate these and
    other endearing traits in their pets, nothing says that the
    cleverness of a highly intelligent primate such as a chim-
    panzee is part of the package. Scientists generally believe
    the reasoning abilities of chimps to be considerably greater
    than that of dogs. But many people nonetheless believe that
    dogs are smarter than chimps precisely because of our
    familiarity and emotional ties with the dogs that we love.
    We apply the same secret rules to our fellow humans: the
    old in-group, out-group story. People in your in-group are
    those who are similar to you, either because they belong
    to the same organizations as you, or enjoy the same
    activities, or, and this is the kicker, because they are simply
    around more often. Dogs, because of their proximity to
    their owners, are definitely in. The intensity of our
    relationship with dogs causes us, quite naturally, to imbue
    them with high-level mental abilities, whether they have
    earned those extra intelligence points or not. We like them,
    so we think well of them.

    Passage 2
    Every dog trainer that I know had the same childhood, a
    childhood filled with the brilliant, heroic dogs of literature.
    We read about dogs who regularly traveled thousands of
    miles to be reunited with owners who somehow misplaced
    them, repeatedly saved people from certain death, and
    continually exhibited a better grasp of strategic problem-
    solving than the average Ph.D. In the preface to one of his
    many dog stories, S. P. Meek a bit shamefacedly remarked
    that in writing of dogs "I endeavored to hold these heroes
    down to the level of canine intelligence, and to make them,
    above all, believable. If at times I seem to have made them
    show supercanine intelligence, it is because my enthusiasm
    has run away with me." We forgave him, of course.
    It was something of a shock, therefore, to discover
    how the learning theory "experts" believed dogs think
    and learn. I was told that dogs, unlike chimpanzees, have
    no real reasoning ability. Dogs don't think: rather, they
    learn to avoid the unpleasant (negative reinforcement), seek
    the pleasant (positive reinforcement), or some combination
    of the two. To contend otherwise was to be guilty of the sin
    of anthropomorphizing, the attribution to an animal of
    motivations and consciousness that only a human being
    could possess.
    Yet as a dog trainer, I find myself siding more with the
    Meeks than I do with the learning theorists: nobody could
    believe dispassionately in the totality of positive and nega-
    tive reinforcement after seeing the pure intelligence shining
    in the face of a border collie intent upon helping a shepherd
    herd sheep. Dogs do think and reason. Granted, a dog might
    not be able to run a maze as quickly as a chimp. But a dog
    outshines any other animal that I know in the ability to
    work willingly with a human being, to communicate with
    a puzzling creature who often makes incomprehensible
    demands. Researchers have increasingly come to view
    intelligence as a complex collection of mental abilities
    that cannot be fully captured in any simple way. Dogs
    are geniuses at being useful, and it is this usefulness
    that we admire when we praise their intelligence. As
    Jonica Newby, a specialist in animal-human interaction,
    writes, "In some ways intelligence is a matter of match-
    ing behavior to environment. To compare intelligence
    in creatures that have evolved differently is a bit like
    deciding which has hit upon the best mode of travel: the
    dolphin or the horse." And it is dogs, not chimps, who
    possess the most helpful mode of travel for human beings.

    ...view full instructions

    Darwin (lines 1-13, Passage 1) and Meek (lines 45-51, Passage 2) serve as examples of
  • Question 4
    1 / -0

    Directions For Questions

    Both passages discuss the issue of the intelligence of dogs.
    Passage 1 was adapted from a 2001 book on animal
    intelligence. Passage 2 was written in 2001 by a dog
    trainer and writer.

    Passage 1
    It was no accident that nineteenth-century naturalist
    Charles Darwin strove to connect the mentality and
    emotionality of people with that of dogs, rather than, say,
    doves or horses. Neither his theory of evolution nor any
    general understanding of biology demanded that he pref-
    erentially underline our similarity to dogs over other
    species. But politically and emotionally, the choice was
    inevitable for an English gentleman who had set himself
    the task of making the idea of evolutionary continuity
    palatable. Darwin wrote that "dogs possess something
    very similar to a conscience. They certainly possess
    some power of self-command. . . . Dogs have long been
    accepted as the very type of fidelity and obedience."
    Darwin was not alone in his beliefs that dogs possess
    human virtues. The characteristics of loyalty and obedience,
    coupled with an expressive face and body, can account
    for why dogs are such popular and valued pets in many
    cultures. Depending on the breed and the individual, dogs
    can be noble, charming, affectionate, and reliable. But
    while all dog owners should rightly appreciate these and
    other endearing traits in their pets, nothing says that the
    cleverness of a highly intelligent primate such as a chim-
    panzee is part of the package. Scientists generally believe
    the reasoning abilities of chimps to be considerably greater
    than that of dogs. But many people nonetheless believe that
    dogs are smarter than chimps precisely because of our
    familiarity and emotional ties with the dogs that we love.
    We apply the same secret rules to our fellow humans: the
    old in-group, out-group story. People in your in-group are
    those who are similar to you, either because they belong
    to the same organizations as you, or enjoy the same
    activities, or, and this is the kicker, because they are simply
    around more often. Dogs, because of their proximity to
    their owners, are definitely in. The intensity of our
    relationship with dogs causes us, quite naturally, to imbue
    them with high-level mental abilities, whether they have
    earned those extra intelligence points or not. We like them,
    so we think well of them.

    Passage 2
    Every dog trainer that I know had the same childhood, a
    childhood filled with the brilliant, heroic dogs of literature.
    We read about dogs who regularly traveled thousands of
    miles to be reunited with owners who somehow misplaced
    them, repeatedly saved people from certain death, and
    continually exhibited a better grasp of strategic problem-
    solving than the average Ph.D. In the preface to one of his
    many dog stories, S. P. Meek a bit shamefacedly remarked
    that in writing of dogs "I endeavored to hold these heroes
    down to the level of canine intelligence, and to make them,
    above all, believable. If at times I seem to have made them
    show supercanine intelligence, it is because my enthusiasm
    has run away with me." We forgave him, of course.
    It was something of a shock, therefore, to discover
    how the learning theory "experts" believed dogs think
    and learn. I was told that dogs, unlike chimpanzees, have
    no real reasoning ability. Dogs don't think: rather, they
    learn to avoid the unpleasant (negative reinforcement), seek
    the pleasant (positive reinforcement), or some combination
    of the two. To contend otherwise was to be guilty of the sin
    of anthropomorphizing, the attribution to an animal of
    motivations and consciousness that only a human being
    could possess.
    Yet as a dog trainer, I find myself siding more with the
    Meeks than I do with the learning theorists: nobody could
    believe dispassionately in the totality of positive and nega-
    tive reinforcement after seeing the pure intelligence shining
    in the face of a border collie intent upon helping a shepherd
    herd sheep. Dogs do think and reason. Granted, a dog might
    not be able to run a maze as quickly as a chimp. But a dog
    outshines any other animal that I know in the ability to
    work willingly with a human being, to communicate with
    a puzzling creature who often makes incomprehensible
    demands. Researchers have increasingly come to view
    intelligence as a complex collection of mental abilities
    that cannot be fully captured in any simple way. Dogs
    are geniuses at being useful, and it is this usefulness
    that we admire when we praise their intelligence. As
    Jonica Newby, a specialist in animal-human interaction,
    writes, "In some ways intelligence is a matter of match-
    ing behavior to environment. To compare intelligence
    in creatures that have evolved differently is a bit like
    deciding which has hit upon the best mode of travel: the
    dolphin or the horse." And it is dogs, not chimps, who
    possess the most helpful mode of travel for human beings.

    ...view full instructions

    In line 53, the author of Passage 2 uses quotation marks to
  • Question 5
    1 / -0

    Directions For Questions

    Both passages discuss the issue of the intelligence of dogs.
    Passage 1 was adapted from a 2001 book on animal
    intelligence. Passage 2 was written in 2001 by a dog
    trainer and writer.

    Passage 1
    It was no accident that nineteenth-century naturalist
    Charles Darwin strove to connect the mentality and
    emotionality of people with that of dogs, rather than, say,
    doves or horses. Neither his theory of evolution nor any
    general understanding of biology demanded that he pref-
    erentially underline our similarity to dogs over other
    species. But politically and emotionally, the choice was
    inevitable for an English gentleman who had set himself
    the task of making the idea of evolutionary continuity
    palatable. Darwin wrote that "dogs possess something
    very similar to a conscience. They certainly possess
    some power of self-command. . . . Dogs have long been
    accepted as the very type of fidelity and obedience."
    Darwin was not alone in his beliefs that dogs possess
    human virtues. The characteristics of loyalty and obedience,
    coupled with an expressive face and body, can account
    for why dogs are such popular and valued pets in many
    cultures. Depending on the breed and the individual, dogs
    can be noble, charming, affectionate, and reliable. But
    while all dog owners should rightly appreciate these and
    other endearing traits in their pets, nothing says that the
    cleverness of a highly intelligent primate such as a chim-
    panzee is part of the package. Scientists generally believe
    the reasoning abilities of chimps to be considerably greater
    than that of dogs. But many people nonetheless believe that
    dogs are smarter than chimps precisely because of our
    familiarity and emotional ties with the dogs that we love.
    We apply the same secret rules to our fellow humans: the
    old in-group, out-group story. People in your in-group are
    those who are similar to you, either because they belong
    to the same organizations as you, or enjoy the same
    activities, or, and this is the kicker, because they are simply
    around more often. Dogs, because of their proximity to
    their owners, are definitely in. The intensity of our
    relationship with dogs causes us, quite naturally, to imbue
    them with high-level mental abilities, whether they have
    earned those extra intelligence points or not. We like them,
    so we think well of them.

    Passage 2
    Every dog trainer that I know had the same childhood, a
    childhood filled with the brilliant, heroic dogs of literature.
    We read about dogs who regularly traveled thousands of
    miles to be reunited with owners who somehow misplaced
    them, repeatedly saved people from certain death, and
    continually exhibited a better grasp of strategic problem-
    solving than the average Ph.D. In the preface to one of his
    many dog stories, S. P. Meek a bit shamefacedly remarked
    that in writing of dogs "I endeavored to hold these heroes
    down to the level of canine intelligence, and to make them,
    above all, believable. If at times I seem to have made them
    show supercanine intelligence, it is because my enthusiasm
    has run away with me." We forgave him, of course.
    It was something of a shock, therefore, to discover
    how the learning theory "experts" believed dogs think
    and learn. I was told that dogs, unlike chimpanzees, have
    no real reasoning ability. Dogs don't think: rather, they
    learn to avoid the unpleasant (negative reinforcement), seek
    the pleasant (positive reinforcement), or some combination
    of the two. To contend otherwise was to be guilty of the sin
    of anthropomorphizing, the attribution to an animal of
    motivations and consciousness that only a human being
    could possess.
    Yet as a dog trainer, I find myself siding more with the
    Meeks than I do with the learning theorists: nobody could
    believe dispassionately in the totality of positive and nega-
    tive reinforcement after seeing the pure intelligence shining
    in the face of a border collie intent upon helping a shepherd
    herd sheep. Dogs do think and reason. Granted, a dog might
    not be able to run a maze as quickly as a chimp. But a dog
    outshines any other animal that I know in the ability to
    work willingly with a human being, to communicate with
    a puzzling creature who often makes incomprehensible
    demands. Researchers have increasingly come to view
    intelligence as a complex collection of mental abilities
    that cannot be fully captured in any simple way. Dogs
    are geniuses at being useful, and it is this usefulness
    that we admire when we praise their intelligence. As
    Jonica Newby, a specialist in animal-human interaction,
    writes, "In some ways intelligence is a matter of match-
    ing behavior to environment. To compare intelligence
    in creatures that have evolved differently is a bit like
    deciding which has hit upon the best mode of travel: the
    dolphin or the horse." And it is dogs, not chimps, who
    possess the most helpful mode of travel for human beings.

    ...view full instructions

    The "experts" (line 53) would most likely argue that which of the following is guilty of the "sin" mentioned in line 58 ?
  • Question 6
    1 / -0

    Directions For Questions

    Both passages discuss the issue of the intelligence of dogs.
    Passage 1 was adapted from a 2001 book on animal
    intelligence. Passage 2 was written in 2001 by a dog
    trainer and writer.

    Passage 1
    It was no accident that nineteenth-century naturalist
    Charles Darwin strove to connect the mentality and
    emotionality of people with that of dogs, rather than, say,
    doves or horses. Neither his theory of evolution nor any
    general understanding of biology demanded that he pref-
    erentially underline our similarity to dogs over other
    species. But politically and emotionally, the choice was
    inevitable for an English gentleman who had set himself
    the task of making the idea of evolutionary continuity
    palatable. Darwin wrote that "dogs possess something
    very similar to a conscience. They certainly possess
    some power of self-command. . . . Dogs have long been
    accepted as the very type of fidelity and obedience."
    Darwin was not alone in his beliefs that dogs possess
    human virtues. The characteristics of loyalty and obedience,
    coupled with an expressive face and body, can account
    for why dogs are such popular and valued pets in many
    cultures. Depending on the breed and the individual, dogs
    can be noble, charming, affectionate, and reliable. But
    while all dog owners should rightly appreciate these and
    other endearing traits in their pets, nothing says that the
    cleverness of a highly intelligent primate such as a chim-
    panzee is part of the package. Scientists generally believe
    the reasoning abilities of chimps to be considerably greater
    than that of dogs. But many people nonetheless believe that
    dogs are smarter than chimps precisely because of our
    familiarity and emotional ties with the dogs that we love.
    We apply the same secret rules to our fellow humans: the
    old in-group, out-group story. People in your in-group are
    those who are similar to you, either because they belong
    to the same organizations as you, or enjoy the same
    activities, or, and this is the kicker, because they are simply
    around more often. Dogs, because of their proximity to
    their owners, are definitely in. The intensity of our
    relationship with dogs causes us, quite naturally, to imbue
    them with high-level mental abilities, whether they have
    earned those extra intelligence points or not. We like them,
    so we think well of them.

    Passage 2
    Every dog trainer that I know had the same childhood, a
    childhood filled with the brilliant, heroic dogs of literature.
    We read about dogs who regularly traveled thousands of
    miles to be reunited with owners who somehow misplaced
    them, repeatedly saved people from certain death, and
    continually exhibited a better grasp of strategic problem-
    solving than the average Ph.D. In the preface to one of his
    many dog stories, S. P. Meek a bit shamefacedly remarked
    that in writing of dogs "I endeavored to hold these heroes
    down to the level of canine intelligence, and to make them,
    above all, believable. If at times I seem to have made them
    show supercanine intelligence, it is because my enthusiasm
    has run away with me." We forgave him, of course.
    It was something of a shock, therefore, to discover
    how the learning theory "experts" believed dogs think
    and learn. I was told that dogs, unlike chimpanzees, have
    no real reasoning ability. Dogs don't think: rather, they
    learn to avoid the unpleasant (negative reinforcement), seek
    the pleasant (positive reinforcement), or some combination
    of the two. To contend otherwise was to be guilty of the sin
    of anthropomorphizing, the attribution to an animal of
    motivations and consciousness that only a human being
    could possess.
    Yet as a dog trainer, I find myself siding more with the
    Meeks than I do with the learning theorists: nobody could
    believe dispassionately in the totality of positive and nega-
    tive reinforcement after seeing the pure intelligence shining
    in the face of a border collie intent upon helping a shepherd
    herd sheep. Dogs do think and reason. Granted, a dog might
    not be able to run a maze as quickly as a chimp. But a dog
    outshines any other animal that I know in the ability to
    work willingly with a human being, to communicate with
    a puzzling creature who often makes incomprehensible
    demands. Researchers have increasingly come to view
    intelligence as a complex collection of mental abilities
    that cannot be fully captured in any simple way. Dogs
    are geniuses at being useful, and it is this usefulness
    that we admire when we praise their intelligence. As
    Jonica Newby, a specialist in animal-human interaction,
    writes, "In some ways intelligence is a matter of match-
    ing behavior to environment. To compare intelligence
    in creatures that have evolved differently is a bit like
    deciding which has hit upon the best mode of travel: the
    dolphin or the horse." And it is dogs, not chimps, who
    possess the most helpful mode of travel for human beings.

    ...view full instructions

    How do the authors of the two passages differ in their assumptions about animal intelligence?
  • Question 7
    1 / -0

    Directions For Questions

    Both passages discuss the issue of the intelligence of dogs.
    Passage 1 was adapted from a 2001 book on animal
    intelligence. Passage 2 was written in 2001 by a dog
    trainer and writer.

    Passage 1
    It was no accident that nineteenth-century naturalist
    Charles Darwin strove to connect the mentality and
    emotionality of people with that of dogs, rather than, say,
    doves or horses. Neither his theory of evolution nor any
    general understanding of biology demanded that he pref-
    erentially underline our similarity to dogs over other
    species. But politically and emotionally, the choice was
    inevitable for an English gentleman who had set himself
    the task of making the idea of evolutionary continuity
    palatable. Darwin wrote that "dogs possess something
    very similar to a conscience. They certainly possess
    some power of self-command. . . . Dogs have long been
    accepted as the very type of fidelity and obedience."
    Darwin was not alone in his beliefs that dogs possess
    human virtues. The characteristics of loyalty and obedience,
    coupled with an expressive face and body, can account
    for why dogs are such popular and valued pets in many
    cultures. Depending on the breed and the individual, dogs
    can be noble, charming, affectionate, and reliable. But
    while all dog owners should rightly appreciate these and
    other endearing traits in their pets, nothing says that the
    cleverness of a highly intelligent primate such as a chim-
    panzee is part of the package. Scientists generally believe
    the reasoning abilities of chimps to be considerably greater
    than that of dogs. But many people nonetheless believe that
    dogs are smarter than chimps precisely because of our
    familiarity and emotional ties with the dogs that we love.
    We apply the same secret rules to our fellow humans: the
    old in-group, out-group story. People in your in-group are
    those who are similar to you, either because they belong
    to the same organizations as you, or enjoy the same
    activities, or, and this is the kicker, because they are simply
    around more often. Dogs, because of their proximity to
    their owners, are definitely in. The intensity of our
    relationship with dogs causes us, quite naturally, to imbue
    them with high-level mental abilities, whether they have
    earned those extra intelligence points or not. We like them,
    so we think well of them.

    Passage 2
    Every dog trainer that I know had the same childhood, a
    childhood filled with the brilliant, heroic dogs of literature.
    We read about dogs who regularly traveled thousands of
    miles to be reunited with owners who somehow misplaced
    them, repeatedly saved people from certain death, and
    continually exhibited a better grasp of strategic problem-
    solving than the average Ph.D. In the preface to one of his
    many dog stories, S. P. Meek a bit shamefacedly remarked
    that in writing of dogs "I endeavored to hold these heroes
    down to the level of canine intelligence, and to make them,
    above all, believable. If at times I seem to have made them
    show supercanine intelligence, it is because my enthusiasm
    has run away with me." We forgave him, of course.
    It was something of a shock, therefore, to discover
    how the learning theory "experts" believed dogs think
    and learn. I was told that dogs, unlike chimpanzees, have
    no real reasoning ability. Dogs don't think: rather, they
    learn to avoid the unpleasant (negative reinforcement), seek
    the pleasant (positive reinforcement), or some combination
    of the two. To contend otherwise was to be guilty of the sin
    of anthropomorphizing, the attribution to an animal of
    motivations and consciousness that only a human being
    could possess.
    Yet as a dog trainer, I find myself siding more with the
    Meeks than I do with the learning theorists: nobody could
    believe dispassionately in the totality of positive and nega-
    tive reinforcement after seeing the pure intelligence shining
    in the face of a border collie intent upon helping a shepherd
    herd sheep. Dogs do think and reason. Granted, a dog might
    not be able to run a maze as quickly as a chimp. But a dog
    outshines any other animal that I know in the ability to
    work willingly with a human being, to communicate with
    a puzzling creature who often makes incomprehensible
    demands. Researchers have increasingly come to view
    intelligence as a complex collection of mental abilities
    that cannot be fully captured in any simple way. Dogs
    are geniuses at being useful, and it is this usefulness
    that we admire when we praise their intelligence. As
    Jonica Newby, a specialist in animal-human interaction,
    writes, "In some ways intelligence is a matter of match-
    ing behavior to environment. To compare intelligence
    in creatures that have evolved differently is a bit like
    deciding which has hit upon the best mode of travel: the
    dolphin or the horse." And it is dogs, not chimps, who
    possess the most helpful mode of travel for human beings.

    ...view full instructions

    Based on lines 63-67 ("nobody . . . sheep"), the author of Passage 2 would most likely appear to the author of Passage 1 as
  • Question 8
    1 / -0
    Choose the option that best summarizes the following:
    The International Center is hosting Spanish Conversation classes. They help non-native speakers of Spanish practice their Spanish speaking skills.
    Solution
    The correct summary to the given sentence is Option D. Summarizing involves retaining the main idea of the given sentence with minimum use of words. Options A and B are incorrect as the sentences are too long to be called a summary, as the summarized sentence is expected to be one third or half of the main sentence. Option C is incorrect as the idea remains incomplete because what classes are being hosted isn't mentioned.
  • Question 9
    1 / -0
    Diana wishes to write a letter to the company for which she works to inform them that she is resigning. Which of the following options is the most appropriate subject for the letter? 
    Solution
    Subject of the letter contains the central idea of any letter. Resignation letter is a kind of a formal letter which one writes to one's supervisor at the time of leaving an organization. So, the subject should define the purpose of the letter. Option C is correct as it clearly denotes the purpose of the letter. The other options are not correct because they do not adhere to the norms of the format. 'Please' suggests request which is usually written in the body. So, option D is incorrect. Here, Diana is issuing her resignation from the company and not from her post. So, option B is incorrect. This is not a 'leave application'. Hence, option A is also incorrect.
  • Question 10
    1 / -0
    Select the option that appropriately summarizes the following:
    There will be a party at Denise's house tomorrow. Would you like to go?
    Solution
    The correct summary of the given sentence is Option C. It includes the whole idea of the sentence that one is asking another if he wants to go to a party that will be held at Denise's house tomorrow. Option A does not give an appropriate idea of the sentence and thus is incorrect. Option B and D do not adhere to the format of summary writing as the tense of the sentences have changed.
Self Studies
User
Question Analysis
  • Correct -

  • Wrong -

  • Skipped -

My Perfomance
  • Score

    -

    out of -
  • Rank

    -

    out of -
Re-Attempt Weekly Quiz Competition
Self Studies Get latest Exam Updates
& Study Material Alerts!
No, Thanks
Self Studies
Click on Allow to receive notifications
Allow Notification
Self Studies
Self Studies Self Studies
To enable notifications follow this 2 steps:
  • First Click on Secure Icon Self Studies
  • Second click on the toggle icon
Allow Notification
Get latest Exam Updates & FREE Study Material Alerts!
Self Studies ×
Open Now